U.S. Department of Transportation
Federal Highway Administration
1200 New Jersey Avenue, SE
Washington, DC 20590
202-366-4000


Skip to content
Facebook iconYouTube iconTwitter iconFlickr iconLinkedInInstagram

Safety

FHWA Home / Safety / Older Users

Handbook for Designing Roadways for the Aging Population

Chapter 10. Construction/Work Zones

The following discussion presents the rationale and supporting evidence for Handbook treatments pertaining to these seven proven and promising practices.

Proven Practices

  1. Signing and Advance Warning
  2. Portable Changeable (Variable) Message Signs
  3. Channelization (Path Guidance)
  4. Delineation of Crossovers/Alternate Travel Paths
  5. Temporary Pavement Markings

Promising Practices

  1. Increased Letter Height for Temporary Work Zone Signs
  2. Work Zone Road Safety Audits

Proven Practices
43. Signing and Advance Warning

Minimum requirements for safely negotiating a lane closure include an awareness of a decrease in pavement width ahead, and of the direction of the lateral shift in the travel path; a detection of traffic control devices marking the location of the lane drop (beginning of taper); a timely decision about the most appropriate maneuver, taking other nearby traffic into account; and smooth vehicle control through maneuver execution. In the vicinity of a lane closure, the longer the information needs supporting these requirements remain unmet for the least capable drivers within the traffic stream, the less likely is a smooth transition through the work area for all drivers (Goodwin, 1975). The more time that is required for aging drivers to prepare and initiate a merging maneuver, the more dense following traffic (including the adjacent lane) is likely to become; this, in turn, will make gap judgments and maneuver decisions at the point of a lane closure more difficult, and will increase the likelihood of erratic vehicle movements resulting in conflicts between motorists.

Relevant alterations in aging adults' cognitive-motor processes include: failure to use advance preparatory information (Botwinick, 1965); difficulty in processing stimuli that are spatially incompatible (Rabbitt, 1968); initiation deficit in dealing with increased task complexity (Jordan and Rabbitt, 1977); and inability to regulate performance speed (Rabbitt, 1979; Salthouse, 1979; Salthouse and Somberg, 1982). Stelmach, Goggin, and Garcia-Colera (1987) found that older adults showed disproportionate response slowing when compared with younger subjects, when there was low expectancy for a required movement. When subjects obtained full information about an upcoming response, reaction time (RT) was faster in all age groups. Stelmach et al. (1987) concluded that aging drivers may be particularly disadvantaged when they are required to initiate a movement in which there is no opportunity to prepare a response. Preparatory intervals and length of precue viewing times are determining factors in age-related differences in movement preparation and planning (Goggin, Stelmach, and Amrhein, 1989). When preparatory intervals are manipulated such that aging adults have longer stimulus exposure and longer intervals between stimuli, they profit from the longer inspection times by performing better and exhibiting less slowness of movement (Eisdorfer, 1975; Goggin et al., 1989). Since aging drivers benefit from longer exposure to stimuli, Winter (1985) proposed that signs should be spaced farther apart to allow drivers enough time to view information and decide which action to take. Increased viewing time will reduce response uncertainty and decrease aging drivers' RT.

Table 55. Cross-references of related entries for signing and advance warning.
Applications in Standard Reference Manuals
MUTCD (2009) NCHRP 500 – Volume 9 (2004) Traffic Engineering Handbook (2009)
Sect. 6C.05

Sect. 6C.06

Sects. 6F.16 & 6F.17

Sects. 6F.81 to 6F.83

Sect. 6F.61

Sect. 6F.63

Sect. 6G.04

Sect. 6G.10

Sects. 6G.12 through 6G.19

Tables 6C-1 & 6C-3

Figs. 6H-3, 6H-5, 6H-6, 6H-10 through 6H-12, 6H-15, 6H-16, 6H-18, 6H-19, & 6H-21 through 6H-46 plus associated notes with each fig.
Pgs. V-8-V-11, Sect. on Strategy 3.1 B1: Provide advance Warning Signs (T)

Pgs. V-26-V-27, Sect. on Strategy 3.1 B11: Improve Traffic Control at Work Zones (T)
Pgs. 357-376, Sects. IIA through IID Pgs. 391-392, Sect. on Older Drivers and Pedestrians

In focus group discussions consisting of 81 drivers ages 65 to 86, pavement width transitions were identified as sources of difficulty by 50 percent of the participants (Staplin, et al., 1997). The drivers participating in these discussions suggested longer merging areas to give them more opportunity to find a safe gap and the use of multiple warning signs to allow them to plan their maneuver at an earlier point upstream. Use of multiple signs to give advance notice of downstream work zones and of required maneuvers was also offered as a desired change by aging drivers participating in an earlier focus group (Staplin, Lococo, and Sim, 1990).

Lyles (1981) conducted studies on two-lane rural roads to evaluate the effectiveness of alternate signing sequences for providing warning to motorists of construction and maintenance activities that required a lane closure. The signs tested included a standard MUTCD warning sequence, the same sequence augmented with continuously flashing warning lights on the signs, and a sequence of symbol signs (WORKER and RIGHT LANE CLOSED). The most effective sign sequence was one that was flasher augmented; this treatment was twice as effective as similar signs with no warning lights in slowing vehicles in the vicinity of the lane closure.

The use of word messages on signs in highway work areas raises sign legibility issues for aging drivers. In research conducted to improve the legibility of the RIGHT/LEFT LANE CLOSED and ROAD CONSTRUCTION series signs using test subjects in three age groups (18–44, 45–64, and 65 and older), Kuemmel (1992) concluded the following for black on orange (negative contrast) signs: (1) signs that increased both letter size and stroke width (SW) while maintaining or increasing the standard alphabet letter series resulted in the best improvement; (2) increasing letter size while decreasing the alphabet series (e.g., from C to B) reduces sign legibility, particularly at night; (3) the use of letter series E, with its 21-percent increase in SW-to-letter height over 8-in series C letters, appears to overcome the problems of irradiation (or overglow phenomenon) with high intensity retroreflective materials, thus increasing night legibility; (4) the legibility distance of the ROAD CONSTRUCTION signs can be increased by changing the word "construction" to "work," and increasing the letter size from 175-mm (7-in) series C to 200-mm (8-in) series C; and (5) for the RIGHT LANE CLOSED series, use of symbol signs will have to supplement word legend signs, and for the CENTER LANE CLOSED series, redundant signs will have to be employed if a 48-in maximum sign size is to be maintained. The author pointed out that the minimum required visibility distance (MRVD) for both signs is 331 ft at 55 mph and 369 ft at 65 mph. The legibility distances obtained in this study for the current standard construction work zone signs ranged from 650 ft for the best observers to 140 ft for the worst observers. In addition, 85th percentile values were closer to the minimum legibility distances than they were to the maximum legibility distances. This finding reinforces the need for redundant signing during the approach to a work zone.

Several studies have centered on the use of fluorescent orange signs for work zone applications, particularly as their increased conspicuity may benefit aging drivers with diminished visual capabilities by providing longer detection distances. Jenssen, et al. (1996) state that fluorescent materials have the potential to increase daytime conspicuity through increased contrast, while the nighttime brightness is sustained through a microprismatic retroreflective system. Burns and Pavelka (1995) explain that the high visibility of fluorescent materials is due to their ability to absorb energy in the near ultraviolet and visible region of the electromagnetic spectrum, and then to re-emit the energy as longer wavelength, visible light. Conventional colorants don't have this property. During the daylight hours from dawn to dusk, there is always sufficient solar energy to elicit light emission from fluorescent materials, irrespective of the cloud cover. Therefore, fluorescent colors maintain a significant daytime visibility advantage over ordinary colors in all types of weather. Improvements in legibility distance have also been found using signs with fluorescent orange microprismatic sheeting (sheeting that provides for high retroreflectance overall, particularly at widest available observation angles).

Chrysler, Carlson, and Hawkins (2002) conducted a controlled field study to determine nighttime sign legibility distance for small ground-mounted signs as a function of retroreflective sheeting type, font, and color. Study methods and results presented here are limited to those describing orange signs. Two font types, both using 6-in black letters were compared, with all signs created using 4-letter words chosen from the MUTCD. The baseline font was Highway Series D. D-Modified font, created for Alabama DOT for use on work zone signs with a thicker stroke width than Highway Series D, was also used. Three types of retroreflective sheeting were used:

Participants in the Chrysler et al. (2002) study included 12 licensed drivers ages 55 to 64 and 12 licensed drivers ages 65 to 75, with males and females equally represented in both groups. Binocular acuity for 21 participants was 20/25 or better, one subject had acuity of 20/30, one had 20/40 acuity, and one had 20/50 acuity. Subjects drove a passenger sedan around a closed course at 30 mph at night using low-beam headlights (HB4 halogen), while attempting to read ground-mounted signs on the right shoulder. Subjects were accompanied by an experimenter who sat in the front passenger seat. Subjects were told to say the word as soon as they could correctly identify it, but were also told that there was no penalty for being wrong, and that it was OK to guess. All signs were offset 14 ft from the right edge line with a height of 8 ft to the center of the sign.

Chrysler et al. (2002) found no significant differences in legibility distance as a function of font type, with the mean of 160 ft for D-Modified, and a mean of 167 ft for Highway Series D. The difference in performance between the two microprismatic sheetings was not significant (Type VIII mean legibility distance = 175 ft and Type IX mean = 169 ft), but both produced significantly longer distances than Type III (mean = 148 ft). There were no significant effects of age on legibility distance for the orange signs. Based on their findings of superior legibility performance with fluorescent microprismatic sheeting, Chrysler et al. (2002) recommended its use for work zone signs over the use of Type III orange sheeting. They also recommended a more conservative legibility index of 33 ft/in (in place of the standard of 40 ft/in) based on their study findings which averaged 24 to 34 feet of legibility per inch of letter height across the 4 sheeting colors evaluated.

Jenssen et al. (1996) conducted a controlled field study using 35 younger subjects (ages 18 to 25) and 44 older subjects (ages 55 to 75) to compare the detection distance, color recognition distance, and the legibility distance of fluorescent signs to traditional signs, under daytime and nighttime conditions. In this study, subjects sat in an open-ended container on a railway car that moved at a speed of 9 mph along a set of unused railroad tracks. Subjects used a response form and were trained to look for specific signs. They activated a response lever that sent a signal to a distance-measuring computer, and then recorded what they observed in the categories provided on their response forms for sign detection, shape, color, symbol, and text. The signs of interest for this discussion were those with an orange background and black text.

Signs with fluorescent orange Type VII retroreflective sheeting were compared to signs with standard orange Type VII retroreflective sheeting, signs with standard orange Type III high intensity grade retroreflective sheeting, and signs with standard orange Type I engineering grade retroreflective sheeting. In the daytime, only signs with Type VII optics were used. The Norwegian town names "LENSVIK," "LAKSVIK," or "LEKSVIK," appeared in randomized order on the signs. The height, angle, and distance of the signs relative to the track were adjusted according to standards for Norwegian two-lane roadways, to ensure realistic viewing positions. Signs were always placed on the right side of the track. For nighttime trials, original headlights for a Volkswagen Golf type 1 vehicle (placed on the railcar at the standard vehicle headlight orientation) were used.

Detection, shape recognition, color recognition, and content recognition were accomplished at significantly greater distances for fluorescent orange retroreflective signs than for the standard orange retroreflective signs, for both younger and older subjects under daytime conditions. The mean detection distance for all subjects during daytime conditions for the fluorescent orange retroreflective signs was 2,697 ft, compared to 2,569 ft for standard orange retroreflective signs. This difference in detection distance was statistically significant. The difference in mean detection distance was larger for the older subjects than for the younger subjects; however, both age groups demonstrated significantly longer detection distances when viewing the fluorescent orange retroreflective signs. The mean shape recognition distance across all subjects during the daytime was 2,441 ft for the fluorescent orange retroreflective signs and 2,136 ft for the standard orange retroreflective signs. Younger subjects were able to correctly recognize the shape of fluorescent signs at an average distance that was 335 ft longer than for the standard signs, and older subjects demonstrated an average shape recognition distance difference of 194 ft. Fluorescent signs also showed significantly longer correct color recognition distances (1,916 ft across age groups) than standard signs (1,539 ft across age groups). Color recognition distances were 429 ft longer for younger subjects, and 350 ft longer for older subjects when viewing the fluorescent signs during the daytime compared to the standard signs. In terms of legibility distances during daytime, across all subjects, the fluorescent signs significantly outperformed the non-fluorescent signs, with a difference in mean legibility distance of 43 ft.

At nighttime, there were no significant differences in detection, shape recognition, color recognition, or contents recognition distances between fluorescent orange retroreflective signs with Type VII sheeting and standard (non-fluorescent) orange retroreflective signs with Type VII sheeting, for either age group. However, comparisons between the three types of retroreflective sheeting indicated that the signs with Type VII sheeting produced detection distances that were 138 ft longer than the signs with high intensity grade sheeting, and 203 ft longer than the signs with engineering grade sheeting, for the older drivers. For the younger drivers, detection distances for the signs with Type VII sheeting were 62 ft longer than those produced by the signs with high intensity grade sheeting, and 118 ft longer than those produced by the signs with engineering grade sheeting.

The mean sign detection distance, shape recognition distance, color recognition distance, and contents recognition distance are presented in Table 56, as a function of subject age group and lighting condition (day vs. night) for the signs with standard orange Type VII sheeting and for the signs with fluorescent orange Type VII sheeting.

Table 56. Subject performance as a function of sheeting type (fluorescent orange Type VII vs. standard orange Type VII), as a function of subject age group and lighting condition (Jenssen, Moen, Brekke, Augdal, and Sjøhaug, 1996).
Lighting Condition Age Group Sign Color (Type VII Sheeting) Mean Distance (m)
Sign Detection Shape Recognition Color Recognition Contents Recognition
Daytime Young Standard Orange 828 707 518 123
Fluorescent Orange 851 808 648 131
Old Standard Orange 726 591 410 92
Fluorescent Orange 776 650 516 108
Nighttime Young Standard Orange 838 707 592 88
Fluorescent Orange 815 623 548 93
Old Standard Orange 771 589 471 92
Fluorescent Orange 758 555 430 66

Burns and Pavelka (1995) conducted a field study using 14 drivers ages 19 to 57 (median age: 40 years), to compare the visibility and conspicuity of durable retroreflective fluorescent sheetings (orange, red, yellow, and yellow-green) to the same color standard highway sheeting (orange, red, yellow, yellow-green, and green), at midday and at dusk. Circular targets with an area measuring 0.01 ft2 were viewed in pairs (one fluorescent and one standard highway color sign) against a 4-ft by 4-ft background. The background consisted of a complex camouflage pattern made up of 23 percent light green, 34 percent green, 25 percent brown, and 18 percent black. The targets were placed 1 ft apart, and were viewed at four distances during the daytime (394 ft, 295 ft, 197 ft, and 98 ft). At dusk (15 min before sunset, and 15 min after sunset), signs were viewed only at 98 ft. Subjects viewed the target pairs while seated in a vehicle that had the headlights turned off. An electronic shutter system provided a viewing duration of 2 s. After each target pair was viewed, subjects provided responses to indicate:

  1. number of targets detected (0, 1, or 2);
  2. target location (left or right);
  3. target color (left color and right color); and
  4. attention-getting value (was one target perceived more easily, or did one target attract your attention more than the other?).

During the daytime, the durable fluorescent targets evaluated in the Burns and Pavelka (1995) study were detected with a higher frequency (close to 100 percent) and at greater distances than the standard highway colors. At midday (facing north on an overcast day) from a distance of 396 ft, 93 percent of the subjects were able to detect the fluorescent orange targets; however, only 43 percent of the subjects could detect the standard orange targets at this distance under the same lighting conditions. At 90 m, 100 percent of the subjects detected the fluorescent signs, compared to 92 percent who detected the standard orange signs. At dusk (15 min after sunset) at a distance of 100 ft, the fluorescent orange signs were detected by 96 percent of the subjects and the standard orange signs were detected by 85 percent of the subjects.

The fluorescent signs also showed greater color recognition than the standard highway signs at all distances. During midday (overcast facing north), the fluorescent orange signs were correctly identified by 61 percent of the subjects at 120 m, 58 percent of the subjects at 90 m, 86 percent of the subjects at 60 m, and 82 percent of the subjects at 30 m. By comparison, the standard orange signs were identified correctly by 7 percent, 23 percent, 64 percent, and 93 percent of the subjects at the same distances. At dusk (15 minutes before sunset) at 30 m, the fluorescent orange signs were correctly identified as orange by 74 percent of the subjects, compared to 62 percent of the subjects for the standard orange signs.

The fluorescent orange sign in each pair of viewings was subjectively determined to be more conspicuous (more attention-getting) than the standard orange highway sign, at 30 m, under all lighting conditions (midday, 15 min before sunset, and 15 min after sunset). Luminance measurements were taken of the targets and their backgrounds, so that a contrast ratio could be calculated. The fluorescent signs always produced a higher contrast ratio than the standard signs. The contrast ratios for the fluorescent orange and standard orange signs are shown in Table 57, under the various, natural lighting conditions utilized in the study.

Table 57. Luminance contrast ratio ([Lt-Lb]/Lb) under different lighting conditions, for fluorescent orange and standard highway orange signs (Burns and Pavelka, 1995).
Color Sign Direction and Lighting Condition
South facing midday—clear North facing midday—overcast North facing dusk—overcast
Fluorescent Orange 5.4 4.4 1.0
Standard Orange 1.8 2.0 0.5

The authors concluded that fluorescent orange signs are more conspicuous than standard highway orange sign colors during the daytime (as were the other fluorescent colors); are detected with higher frequency; and are recognized with greater accuracy at farther distances. Fluorescent signs provide a greater contrast with the background scene, and therefore should be considered as a countermeasure to address problems that aging drivers have in the detection and recognition of traffic signs when viewed against a cluttered background.

Finally, Hummer and Scheffler (1999) conducted a field study at seven long-term work zones in North Carolina with left lane drops on multilane highways, to determine whether the increase in the conspicuity of fluorescent orange signs leads to positive operational changes in driver behavior. All seven sites were left lane drops on four-lane highways (with standard lane and shoulder widths), with the following sequence of orange signs (in pairs, with one sign on each side of the highway):

Six sites had 55 mph speed limits and wide grassy medians, and one site had a 65 mph speed limit. Before the study was conducted, both treatment and control sites existed as work zones with standard orange signs, except for the first sign in the pair, which was fluorescent. In the "before" period, five operational measures were collected on this standard set of signs. In the "after" period, the standard orange signs were replaced with fluorescent signs (same size and message) at the "treatment sites," and the same measures were recorded at these treatment sites, as well as at the "control sites" where the standard signs were left in place. Two weeks elapsed before data were collected in the "after" period, to eliminate novelty effects. The operational measures included:

  1. traffic conflicts (one vehicle brakes or swerves to avoid hitting another);
  2. percentage of all vehicles in the left lane;
  3. percentage of trucks in the left lane;
  4. mean speed; and
  5. speed variance.

These data were collected at the beginning of the taper, at the BEGIN WORK ZONE sign, and at the midpoint of the approach.

With regard to traffic conflicts, a reduction from 153 to 136 at the treatment sites (with fluorescent signs) was observed in the before and after periods, respectively, while an increase from 160 to 187 was observed at the control sites (with standard signs) in the before and after periods. This reduction in conflict frequency was statistically significant, when sites without potential confounding factors were removed from the analysis. Regarding the number of vehicles in the left lane, there was a significant reduction in the percentage of vehicles at the midpoint of the work zone approach at the sites with fluorescent signs (more than a 5 percent reduction, or 100+ fewer vehicles); similarly, an increase in the percentage of trucks that moved out of the left lane before the midpoint (16 more trucks, or 30 percent more than expected) and at the taper (12 more trucks than expected) was observed at the sites with fluorescent signs, compared to sites with the standard orange signs.

Differences in mean speeds were not statistically significant; speeds increased by approximately 1 mph at the midpoint and taper of treatment sites, and decreased by the same amounts at the control sites. However, speed variance decreased at the midpoint and at the taper of the treatment sites (with fluorescent signs), relative to speeds monitored at the control sites (with standard orange signs).

Hummer and Scheffler (1999) state that the operational changes documented during their study would translate to fewer collisions in work zones that display fluorescent orange signs compared to those that display the standard orange signs. Although the overall reduction in traffic conflicts in this study was small (approximately 7 percent), they recommend that agencies use fluorescent orange sheeting on warning signs in work zones similar to those studied, as well as for work zones where warning drivers is as critical or more critical than it was in the current study. These would include long-term work zones where there is flagging, temporary traffic signals, sharp lane shifts, and changed merging patterns, as well as in many temporary and moving work zones. Hummer and Scheffler (1999) state that fluorescent orange sheeting costs only a few dollars more per sign installation than standard orange sheeting, and even if work zone collision frequencies declined by only one or two percent, the benefit-to-cost relationship would be substantial.

Next, a number of studies have been performed to determine the effectiveness and motorist comprehension of static signs and changeable message signs (CMS's)—also referred to as variable message signs (VMS's)—for lane closures. A general indication of the importance of CMS's to accomplish lane control in advance of work zones is provided by a field study on a four-lane section of I-35 in San Antonio conducted by Dudek, Richards, and Faulkner (1981) to evaluate the effects of CMS messages on lane changes at a work-zone lane closure. The measure of effectiveness used to evaluate the CMS was the percentage of vehicles that remained in the closed (median) lane as traffic progressed toward the cone taper. The results indicated that the CMS did encourage drivers to vacate or avoid the closed lane, compared with driver responses at the same site without use of the CMS. The percent volumes in the closed lane were significantly lower when a lane-closure message was displayed than during periods when the sign was blank. Specifically, there was a 46 percent greater reduction in the lane volume attributable to the CMS.

During the conduct of field studies for NCHRP project 3-21(2), the relative proportions of traffic in the through and closed lanes approaching construction lane closures were observed for a sample of more than 196,500 vehicles (Transportation Research Board, 1981). Data gathered in Georgia, Colorado, and California were used to compare these lane distributions between baseline (no CMS) conditions and various CMS applications. A fourth data set, gathered in South Carolina, was used to determine relative effects between certain CMS message alternatives (i.e., speed and closure, speed and merge, closure and merge advisories), and various placement configurations (i.e., one CMS at 2,000 ft in advance; or one CMS at 3,960-ft advance placement; or two CMS devices, one at each advance location; or one CMS placed at 3,960 ft in advance of the taper and an additional arrow panel at the 2,000-ft location). Findings indicated increased preparatory lane change activity, smoother lane-change profiles, and significantly fewer "late exits" (exit from a closed lane within 100 ft of closure) in locations where a CMS was applied at the 3,960-ft advance location and an arrow panel at the 2,000-ft location.

Additional studies of flashing arrow panels at construction sites have shown that they are effective in shifting approaching traffic out of a closed lane (Bates, 1974; Shah and Ray, 1976; Graham, Migletz, and Glennon, 1978; Bryden, 1979; Faulkner and Dudek, 1981). These studies found that arrow panels were effective because they promote early merging into the open lane and fewer vehicles remained in the closed lane at the start of the lane-closure taper. A basis thus exists to assert that a CMS used to give advance notice of the need to exit a lane, followed by the application of an arrow panel, would be of clear benefit to drivers with diminished capabilities resulting from aging, inattentiveness, or transient impairment (e.g., due to fatigue, alcohol, or drugs). While the specific location of the arrow panel in this approach should be consistent with the signing sequence indicated in the MUTCD Part 6H (Figure 6H-33 for divided highways), placement at the beginning of the taper is suggested by the findings reported above.

Mace, Finkle, and Pennak (1996) conducted a static and a dynamic field study to determine the minimum and optimum lamp intensities needed for arrow panel legibility (left arrow or chevron vs. right arrow or chevron presentation) during the day, and minimum and maximum intensity for nighttime operations to minimize glare effects. The authors cite the work of Faulkner and Dudek (1982), who found that sight distances to arrow panels (AP's) influences driver behavior, such that when AP's are used too far in advance of a lane closure, (e.g., 4,000 ft, drivers tend to return to a vacated lane. Also, if sight distance is less than 1,500 ft, an advance supplemental AP is desirable. While no data exist to document problems in the safe use of AP's by aging drivers, the following recommendations (see Table 58) suggested by Mace et al. (1996) for arrow panel lamp intensity provide a useful reference for practitioners. These values ensure visibility for DSD's of 1,500 and 930 ft, for high-speed and low-speed roadways, respectively.

Table 58. Recommended minimum on- and off-axis lamp intensities of arrow panels to ensure daytime visibility by aging drivers, and the maximum intensity recommended for nighttime operations to ensure safe levels of discomfort and disability glare for aging drivers, for high-speed (≥45 mph) and low-speed (<45 mph) roadways (Mace, Finkle, and Pennak, 1996).
Situation Luminous Intensity Requirements (cd per lamp)
Minimum On-Axis Minimum Off-Axis Maximum Hot Spot
Low-Speed Day 300 60 n/a
High-Speed Day 500 100 n/a
Low-Speed Night 90 18 370
High-Speed Night 150 30 370

A questionnaire also was completed during the conduct of NCHRP Project 3-21(2), by 489 subjects ranging in age from under 20 to 80 to gather measures of driver detection, recognition, and comprehension of the CMS devices. Twenty percent of the drivers were age 60 and older. Five tested message conditions were:

  1. speed and closure advisory (MAX SPEED 45 MPH/RIGHT LANE CLOSED);
  2. speed and merge advisory (MAX SPEED 45 MPH/MERGE LEFT);
  3. merge and closure advisory (RIGHT LANE CLOSED/MERGE LEFT);
  4. speed advisory only (SLOW TO 45 MPH); and
  5. closure advisory only (RIGHT LANE CLOSED AHEAD).

Drivers consistently reported that the speed advisory and lane closure message combination was most helpful, was the easiest to read, best met their information needs, and would be most likely to cause them to change lanes early and reduce speed.

A human factors laboratory study was conducted to determine which CMS message alternatives would be most likely to enhance motorists' compliance with lane control messages in work zones (Gish, 1995). The subjects were divided into two age groups consisting of 24 subjects each: the youngest drivers had a mean age of 23.1 years (range: 16–33), and the oldest drivers had a mean age of 70.2 years (range: 65–84). The results of this study indicated that older drivers were more likely to reduce their speed and change lanes than the younger drivers, and that both older and younger drivers' compliance with lane-change messages was strongly influenced by surrounding vehicles and by the visibility of the lane closures themselves, which exerts a strong influence on message credibility. Other factors, such as traffic density, static displays, and merge arrows (arrow panels), influence driver compliance with CMS messages. To optimize lane-change compliance, Gish (1995) recommended that static displays, merge arrows, and other devices be used in addition to CMS messages. A need to study the long-term effectiveness on nonstandard messages was also indicated, and potential improvements in work-zone safety and operations through the use of condition-responsive (real-time) traffic control systems that provide continuously updated information to motorists (for enhanced credibility) were identified.

44. Portable Changeable (Variable) Message Signs

The effectiveness of changeable message signs (CMSs), gauged in terms of observable driver behaviors that traffic management procedures are designed to elicit, rests upon a set of reasonably well-understood human factors. A motorist information system must be rational, relevant, and reliable. Driver sensory/perceptual and cognitive capabilities must be thoughtfully considered to ensure that a message will be acquired and then understood, recalled, and applied by the driver within a desired timeframe; the message must seem to clearly apply to the driver and to reflect current conditions to be credible; and it must be accurate in describing what the driver experiences downstream. The credibility of a highway advisory message certainly depends in part upon a presentation strategy that is "rational," but it also must be perceived to be relevant to the individual motorist, and reliable to the point of being virtually error-free. Reliability requirements—being dependent on real-time data on operations as input to the traffic control system—are most difficult to meet, but probably the most important if high rates of compliance in drivers' vehicle control decisions are ever to be realized.

A motorist's ability to use highway information is governed by: (1) information acquisition, or how well the source can be seen or heard; and (2) information processing, or the speed and accuracy with which the message content can be understood, and its ease of recall by the motorist after message presentation is completed.

In the acquisition of CMS information, a visual task, the key factors are: (1) its conspicuity, or "attention-getting value" to the motorist; (2) the size, brightness (contrast), stroke width-to-height ratio, and spacing of individual characters of text, which together determine the legibility of the message; (3) the placement of the CMS device—overhead versus one side versus both sides of the highway—which affects its likelihood of being blocked from a motorist's view by other vehicles, as well as the "eyes away from the road" time required to fixate upon the message; and (4) the exposure time, or available viewing time, of each message phase presented on a CMS.

Conspicuity is generally not a problem for any type of CMS under low-traffic volumes, although under high volumes with a significant mix of heavy vehicles, a motorist may fail to notice a roadside device because of obscuration. Good conspicuity is achieved by overhead devices under all conditions. While the attention value of a CMS display can be maximized by flashing operations, research by Dudek et al. (2006) indicates this strategy may detract from message comprehension, and is thus discouraged as a standard operating procedure. In rare circumstances, for a unit of information deemed particularly critical by the highway authority, the flashing of a single text element within a message at a slow rate may be justified. Also, if the CMS in question always has some type of message displayed, then slowly flashing (e.g., two cycles per phase) the problem statement line only may be warranted to attract attention. A preferred strategy under such circumstances would be to activate a flashing warning light separate from, though clearly attached to, the CMS.

Table 59. Cross-references of related entries for portable changeable (variable) message signs.
Applications in Standard Reference Manuals
MUTCD (2009) NCHRP 500 – Volume 9 (2004) Traffic Engineering Handbook (2009)
Chapter 2L

Sects. 6F.60 & 6F.61

Figs. 6H-4, 6H-17, 6H-22, 6H-23, 6H-24, 6H-30 through 6H-35, 6H-37 through 6H-39, 6H-42, & 6H-44 plus associated notes with each fig.
Pgs. V-26-V-27, Sect. on Strategy 3.1 B11: Improve Traffic Control at Work Zones (T) Pg. 368, Para. 7

Pg. 371, Sect. on Changeable Message Signs

If it is standard policy to leave the signs blank, then the mere display of a message will capture the driver's attention, without the need to resort to flashing elements. Indeed, driving simulator study findings argue against the use of flashing message elements on CMS's with rare exceptions, as noted above (Dudek, Schrock, Ullman, and Chrysler, 2006). By extension, the use of these devices to display any content except highway safety advisories or traffic control messages should be universally prohibited.

The legibility of a CMS is influenced by the same factors influencing character and message legibility of static signs, including the key factor of driver visual performance capability. Letter acuity declines during adulthood (Pitts, 1982) and aging adults' loss in acuity is accentuated under conditions of low contrast, low luminance, and where there is crowding of visual contours (Sloane, Owsley, Nash, and Helms, 1987; Adams, Wong, Wong, and Gould, 1988). In any event, the legibility for current CMS's is determined primarily by the technology and the device configuration (numbers of rows, characters per row, and number, size, and spacing of pixels per character) as fabricated by a given manufacturer, and for all practical purposes can be treated as a fixed factor—modified by environmental considerations—in considering whether a particular system as implemented in the field will meet motorists' needs.

For any given speed, aging drivers' needs dictate a legibility distance that permits the entire CMS message to be read twice in its entirety. As a general rule, at least 1,000 ft of legibility distance for a motorist with 20/40 visual acuity should be provided on a 55 mph facility. Of the studies that assessed various character matrix forms (number of elements per character cell), most found a 7 x 9 element matrix to be necessary when using lowercase letters, because of the descenders and ascenders, but a 5 x 7 font was generally deemed acceptable with uppercase-only lettering. The MUTCD recommends a minimum legibility requirement of 0.5 mi for trailer&#8211; or large-truck-mounted Portable Changeable Message Signs, and a minimum letter height of 18 in (Section 6F.60). Given that the most common format for a portable sign is 18-in tall characters arranged in three lines of eight characters, this provides for a legibility distance of 147 ft/in of letter height. The MUTCD provides for the use of smaller letter heights (minimum of 10 in) on CMSs mounted on service patrol trucks, provided that the message is legible from at least 650 ft. This provides for a legibility distance of 65 ft/in. Other variables found to significantly affect CMS legibility for aging observers are font, letter width-to-height ratio, contrast orientation, letter height, case, and stroke width (Jenkins, 1991; Mace, Garvey, and Heckard, 1994). The most consistent finding across studies evaluating CMS design elements was that the results found for aging drivers were quantitatively but not qualitatively different from those of their younger counterparts. That is, if a manipulation of a variable resulted in an improved score for younger observers, it almost invariably improved aging observer performance.

Garvey and Mace (1996) conducted several laboratory and controlled field studies to determine optimum legibility requirements of CMS's, particularly for aging drivers. The laboratory studies included 24 "young" subjects ages 16 to 40 (mean age: 26.6); 25 "old" subjects ages 62 to 73 (mean age: 67.9); and 21 "old-old" subjects age 74 and older (mean age: 77.2). The first laboratory study used a CMS simulator that was programmed on a PC, simulating nighttime viewing conditions. Only positive contrast signs (light letters against a dark background) were used. The objectives were: (1) to determine the optimum width-to-height ratio (W:H) and stroke-width-to height ratio (SW:H); (2) to identify the CMS font that produced the smallest size legibility thresholds; and (3) to determine the effect of color on legibility. Six different sizes of each sign were evaluated. The dependent variable in the study was the threshold size at which a character became legible, which was converted into a legibility index (LI) reported in ft/in (m/cm). Seven combinations of CMS matrix size, W:H, and SW:H combinations were evaluated to determine the optimum character legibility, as shown in Table 60.

Table 60. Variables evaluated by Garvey and Mace (1996) using a CMS simulator to determine the optimum character legibility.
Matrix Size Width-to-Height Ratio (W:H) Stroke-Width-to-Height Ratio (SW:H)
5 x 7 1.0 0.13
5 x 7 0.8 0.13
5 x 7 0.7 0.13
15 x 15 1.0 0.13
15 x 15 1.0 0.20
12 x 15 0.8 0.13
12 x 15 0.8 0.20

Results indicated that for all conditions, the younger group performed significantly better (smaller letter size required for legibility) than both older groups, and the "old" group performed better than the "old-old" group. The authors indicated that, generally, what worked well for one age group worked well for all ages. Across all age groups, increasing the width-to-height ratio (W:H) of a character from 0.7 to 1.0 increased the legibility index (LI) by 7 ft/in. This provides an advantage of 38 meters of legibility for the wider letter when using an 18 in letter height, or 1.5 s at 55 mph. A significant stroke-width-to height (SW:H) effect was also found. For the narrow letters (W:H = 0.8), a thinner stroke performed better than a wider stroke by 5 ft/in. This effect was not significant with wider letters. There were no significant differences in legibility index as a function of matrix density. Therefore, for uppercase letters, increasing the number of elements beyond the standard 5 x 7 format did not improve legibility. The authors state that a typical CMS font with a W:H of 1.0 and a SW:H of 0.13 is optimal for the three age groups studied, from the median to the 85th percentile observer. Their data indicate that the 85th percentile old-old observer was capable of reading such a letter at the LI typically expected of CMS's (35 ft/in).

In another laboratory study using the same subjects and test apparatus, Garvey and Mace (1996) found that the fonts typically used by CMS manufacturers performed well, with the exception of "double stroke" characters within a 5 x 7 character matrix. A double-stroke font provided a LI of 43 ft/in for young observers compared to 57 ft/in for the typical CMS font. For old-old observers, the double stroke font provided a LI of 32 ft/in compared to the typical CMS font that provided 38 ft/in. Across all age groups, the double-stroke font resulted in a decrement in LI of 10 ft/in.

In the final laboratory study of CMS character legibility, Garvey and Mace (1996) found significant effects of contrast orientation on letter legibility. Positive-contrast stimuli (lighter colored letters on a dark background) produced a LI of 12 ft/in higher than negative-contrast stimuli (dark letters on a lighter background). This improvement is equal to an additional 220 ft of legibility distance for an 18-in letter height, or 2.75 s at 55 mph. White-on-black signs performed similarly to yellow-on-black signs, except for the highest-percentile old-old group, where yellow-on-black signs were significantly better than white-on-black. Red-on-black signs performed as well as the other two colors for the young observers, but were found to be significantly less legible than yellow or white on black signs for both groups of older observers. The authors point out that the reduced performance of the color red for aging subjects is likely due to its lower luminance, and as people age, they become more sensitive to changes in target luminance. The obtained LI by sign color and observer age and percentile is shown in Table 61.

Table 61. Legibility index obtained by Garvey and Mace (1996) in a laboratory study of CMS character legibility, as a function of driver age, sign color, and percent of drivers accommodated.
Driver Age Percent Accommodated Legibility Index (ft/in)
Yellow on Black White on Black Red on Black
16-40 50 61.6 61.6 63.3
75 58.3 55.0 56.6
85 48.3 51.6 53.3
90 45.0 45.0 46.6
95 40.0 43.3 35.0
62-73 50 53.3 55.0 48.3
75 48.3 50.0 40.0
85 46.6 43.3 38.2
90 43.3 43.3 38.2
95 35.0 41.6 31.6
74+ 50 45.0 45.0 40.0
75 41.6 38.2 33.3
85 40.0 33.3 30.0
90 35.0 31.6 28.3
95 30.0 30.0 30.0

In a dynamic field study, Garvey and Mace (1996) employed older and younger drivers to evaluate legibility distance and detection distance of six portable CMS's. Participants included 33 "young" subjects ages 19 to 40; 25 "old" subjects ages 59 to 72; and 26 "old-old" subjects ages 73 to 82. Other independent variables included contrast orientation (positive or negative); character height (18 in or 42 in); lighting condition (backlit, frontlit, overcast, or rain); character luminance—day (350, 570, 850, or 1,200 cd/m2); character luminance—night (30, 80, 130, 200, 570, 1,200 cd/m2); inter-letter spacing—night (single or double); and sign lighting—night (internal vs. external or backlight vs. LED).

Significant findings in the field study included the following:

Next, the "target value," legibility, and viewing comfort of light-emitting diodes (LEDs) and fiber-optic CMS technologies were compared with flip-disk and conventional overhead guide signs in a field study conducted by Upchurch, et al. (1991). Younger (ages 18 to 31) and older (ages 60 to 79) subjects in this study demonstrated mean daytime target values for fiber-optic, LED, and flip-disk technologies that all were significantly better (longer) than the values for conventional overhead signs. Under nighttime conditions, however, the poorest performance (shortest distances) was demonstrated by both age groups for the flip-disk technology, falling below the conventional sign values as well. The fiber-optic and LED signs again exceeded the conventional signs, based on nighttime mean target value, with the fiber-optic technology showing a slight superiority for aging drivers. Under backlight (sun behind sign) and washout (sun behind driver) conditions, target values for all sign types decreased substantially and the differences among sign types diminished, but the fiber-optic technology still resulted in the best overall performance, across age groups.

Legibility distance results tended to favor the conventional signs, followed by the fiber-optic signs, LED signs, and flip-disk technology. Mean daytime legibility distances for each sign type in this study were as follows: fiber-optic—61 ft/in; LED—42 ft/in; flip-disk—39 ft/in and conventional—88 ft/in. Under nighttime conditions, the conventional signs again could be read at the longest mean distances, followed closely by the fiber-optic and LED signs, with the flip-disk technology showing the poorest performance. Backlight conditions favored the fiber-optic technology, and washout conditions favored the conventional signs; in both cases, however, the flip-disk technology resulted in the shortest legibility distances. Using a threshold for minimal acceptable legibility distance of 628 ft, the study concluded that flip-disk signs are deficient under all conditions except midday daytime viewing, LED signs are deficient under both backlight and washout sun conditions, and fiber-optic signs are deficient only with the sun glare present under backlight conditions.

Mean discomfort ratings were consistent with these patterns of results. Fiber-optic and conventional signs were assigned the best (lowest discomfort) ratings under daytime conditions, by younger and older drivers alike. LED signs caused slightly more discomfort for aging subjects, and flip-disk signs resulted in the highest discomfort ratings, especially for aging drivers. Under nighttime conditions, only the flip-disk technology resulted in high discomfort ratings. Discomfort ratings were more even, and much higher, across sign types under backlight conditions where the sun was behind the sign, though flip-disk signs still were rated the worst by both age groups. Under washout conditions, subjects reported little discomfort for either the fiber-optic or conventional signs, but much greater and roughly equivalent levels of discomfort with the LED and flip-disk technologies.

Table 62 contains legibility distances from the Upchurch et al. (1991) study. For aging drivers, the legibility distances are lower due to the well-documented degradation of visual performance with age. Unfortunately, this is the only study that has assessed legibility distances for aging observers. The legibility distances for conventional bulb matrix and LED/flip-disk hybrid CMS's were estimated from the results of the Upchurch data and data cited in Dudek (1991).

Table 62. Day and night predicted legibility distances for various sign technologies (Upchurch et al., 1991).
Sign Technology (Character Height) Daytime Legibility Distances Nighttime Legibility Distances
Younger Observers Older Observers Younger Observers Older Observers
Fiber-optic 16 in 1,006 ft 959 ft 687 ft 667 ft
Light-emitting diodes 17.8 in * 812 ft 681 ft 794 ft 602 ft
Flip-disk 18 in 731 ft 667 ft 363 ft 348 ft
Bulb matrix 18 in 800 ft 671 ft 750 ft 569 ft
Hybrid LED/flip-disk 18 in 731 ft 667 ft 794 ft 602 ft

* Legibility distance of this technology decreases over time, because as LED's age, they become less bright.

The aging driver legibility distances in Table 62 should be assumed to represent the legibility distances for the various types of technology represented. This ensures that the needs of aging drivers have been met. The results suggest that flip-disk CMS's should not be used at night along roadways where average speeds reach or exceed about 55 mph.

Although the bulb matrix CMS was assessed by Upchurch et al. (1991), no legibility distances for that sign were reported. Legibility distances for this type of CMS have been obtained; however, it is unknown whether any aging observers have been used in assessing legibility distances. Dudek (1991) cited a study in which bulb matrix CMS's provided legibility distances of 800 ft during the day and 750 ft at night. These distances are similar to the legibility distances obtained by Upchurch et al. (1991) for LED-type CMS's using younger observers. Until psychophysical data can be obtained for aging observers viewing bulb matrix signs, the legibility distances for aging observers are assumed to be roughly 671 ft during the day and 569 ft at night. These estimates are based on applying the ratio of older-to-younger legibility distances for the LED-type display.

There are also a number of hybrid CMS's that were not included in the Upchurch et al. study. Hybrid CMS's apply various combinations of sign technologies listed in Table 62 within a single sign. Product literature for one manufacturer's hybrid LED/flip-disk sign states that the sign provides 900 ft of legibility distance during the day and greater than 900 ft at night, using character heights of 18 in. Unfortunately, the methods used to obtain these legibility distances are unknown. Since the sign uses the reflective flip-disk technology during daytime and the LED's at night, the legibility distances for aging observers for the daytime flip-disk in Table 62 (667 ft) should be used as a more realistic estimate of legibility distance with LED/flip-disk hybrids. For nighttime viewing, use the nighttime LED legibility distance (602 feet) in Table 62.

CMS placement affects information acquisition under heavy traffic conditions where a center lane driver's view of a roadside device may be obscured for lengthy intervals. If a facility has more than two lanes, a consideration may be given to placement of a portable CMS in the median—space permitting and where glare from opposing vehicles is absent or minimal due to a large glare angle—rather than on the right shoulder, since lane control practices for heavy trucks are common throughout many corridors. Aging drivers participating in focus groups have reported difficulty seeing portable changeable message signs positioned on the shoulders of multilane roadways, unless they are driving in the right lane. This suggests that signs be elevated as high as possible so that they can be seen across multiple lanes of traffic, and that multiple CMS's be used with the same message — the first to attract attention, and the second further downstream to communicate the message (Kihl, 2005; Kihl, et al., 2004).

A motorist's reading time for a CMS message dictates the required exposure time at a given speed. Exposure time is the length of time a driver is within the legibility distance of the message. The minimum recommended exposure time per page (phase) for a three-line CMS is 3 s, aside from a consideration of any particular set of driver characteristics. However, while some jurisdictions have selected briefer exposure times, the increasing numbers of aging drivers on limited-access highways makes an even stronger case for the 3 s minimum per page. Reading time is the time it actually takes a driver to read a sign message. In instrumented vehicle studies conducted in light traffic with familiar drivers on a rural freeway, reading times averaged 1 to 1.5 s per unit of information (Mast and Ballas, 1976). Reading times under "loaded" driving conditions would be higher, such as under extreme geometry, heavy traffic volumes, large volume of truck traffic, traffic conflicts, or poor climatological conditions. Field research using unfamiliar drivers has indicated that a minimum exposure time of 1 s per short word (four to eight characters) or 2 s per unit of information, whichever is larger, should be used (Carvell, Turner, and Dudek, 1978; Messer, Stockton, and Mounce, 1978; Weaver, et al., 1978; Dudek, et al., 1981). A unit of information is a data item given in a message, which can answer one of the following questions: (1) what happened? (2) where? (3) what is the effect on traffic? (4) for whom is the advisory intended? and (5) what driver action is advised? Thus, the exposure time for a three-line message could vary from 3 s to as much as 6 s, with each phase of a portable CMS at the lower end of this range and with each permanent CMS phase (page) at the upper end, due to differences in the number of characters per line. Reducing the exposure time per phase is warranted only when information is being repeated. For example, a three-line message may be displayed for only 2.5 s if it is a second phase of a two-phase message which repeats one or two lines from the first phase. If the second phase presents new information, the recommended minimum exposure time for both phases remains 3 s.

For a given operating speed, exposure will increase with increasing legibility distance. For example, an overhead sign message that is legible at 650 ft will be exposed to drivers traveling at 55 mph for approximately 8 s. With a legibility distance of 1,000 ft, the message will be exposed for about 12 s. Permanent CMSs generally have legibility distances in the higher range of 900–1,200 ft. However, there is a point at which a sign becomes unreadable during a driver's approach to a CMS, which reduces the legibility distance, particularly for side-mounted CMSs. This unreadable distance, which is dependent on the number of lanes and the sign technology, as well as how far the sign is set back from the roadway edge or how high above the roadway it is mounted, ranges from 280 ft to 420 ft. In an existing system, therefore, required exposure times dictate the maximum length of message that can be displayed, and in all cases, it is desirable that motorists be able to read the entire message on an (unobstructed) CMS twice.

The calculated maximum exposure duration of a message should not exceed 9 s. For two-phase messages, a separate requirement is needed to meet the needs of drivers. In this case, 3 s is added to the required exposure time because of the asynchrony between the time the driver can read the CMS and the onset of CMS phase displayed. In other words, the phase that the driver reads initially may have already been displayed for 2 s by the time he or she can read it. Thus, the driver will not have enough time to read this phase and will need to view that phase again. The net result is that 3 s needs to be added to the required exposure time to allow drivers to read the phase that first came into view a second time. Since the maximum recommended exposure time is 9 s, only 6 s of actual message reading time is allowed on a two-phase CMS, whereas the full 9 s can be used for a single-phase message. The important point here is that single-phase messages can more efficiently convey information to drivers. When use of a single-phase CMS is not possible because of message length, multiple devices with a single phase on each device will be superior to multiple phases on a single device. Part 6 of the 2009 MUTCD (Section 6F.60) states that when multiple portable changeable message signs are needed, they should be placed on the same side of the roadway and they should be separated from each other by a distance of at least 1,000 feet on freeways and expressways, and by a distance of at least 500 feet on other types of highways.

For these reasons, the maximum number of phases used to display a message on a permanent CMS should be two. The most effective format for CMS message presentation is a single phase which consists of a maximum of three units of information, but if two phases are required, each should be worded so that it can stand alone and still be understood. Portable CMS devices, though limited to fewer characters per line, should also be restricted to two phases. At high speeds (55 mph), a driver may only have 2.8 to 4.6 s to read a message on a side-mounted CMS, depending on the available legibility distance. For this reason, messages should be restricted to one phase at high speeds.

Recommendations against the use of flashing messages and message elements have been made, based on the findings of a driving simulator study using older and younger drivers (Dudek, et al., 2006). In this study, 64 drivers ages 18 to 80 viewed CMS messages presented on the right side of a simulated roadway, while following a lead vehicle that varied its speed before, during, and after the display of a CMS, as well as at times when there was no CMS. One-phase, three-line messages were presented on the CMS in one of three formats: 1) all three lines flashing; (2) flashing the top line of a three-line message; and (3) all three lines static. The flash rate for the dynamic messages was 1.5 s on and 0.5 s off. There were no significant differences in average reading time between signs with all three lines flashing (7.2 s) and static messages (7.2 s), but average reading time for messages where the top line flashed were significantly longer than for static messages (7.8 s vs. 7.1 s). Comprehension for three-line flashing messages was lower (but not significantly) than for three-line static messages, for subjects' first exposure to the flashing display, which suggests that flashing all three lines may have an adverse effect on drivers unfamiliar with this mode of CMS display. Comprehension was even lower on signs where only the top line flashed, but the differences were not statistically significant. Significantly more participants preferred the static message over the three-line flashing message (60% to 40%). The most common reason cited by participants who preferred the flashing mode was that it caught their attention. The most common reason for those who preferred a static message was that it gave the driver more time to read the message and was easier to read. Participants were evenly split (50-50) in their preference for three-line static vs. first line flashing on a three-line message, but opponents of the first-line flashing message indicated that the flashing line was distracting. There were no significant differences in driver performance measures collected in the simulator (e.g., acceleration noise, lane positioning, or headway) as a function of message display format. There were no age effects on reading time, message comprehension, display preference, or driver performance.

The motorist's need for rapid understanding and integration of message components also focuses attention on the formatting of multiword text displays. The main concern is with "units of information" (i.e., where and how to divide phrases) and with the use of abbreviations and contractions in CMS messages. These formatting issues are discussed below.

Work zones constitute driving situations that require a high amount of controlled processing, and data show that cognitive ability scores that measure processing efficiency decline with age (Ackerman, 1987). In fact, sensory memory, working memory, and divided attention all show a decline with aging and must be considered in the display of messages on CMSs. This reinforces the conclusion that a message should be limited to a single phase, or certainty no more than two, because multiple phases will interfere with message comprehension. There is also considerable evidence that aging adults have poorer working memory function than younger adults (Salthouse, 1991; Salthouse and Babcock, 1991). This indicates that message length should be limited to the fewest, most relevant units possible.

Finally, aging adults are particularly disadvantaged when they are required to use working memory to manage multiple tasks (Ponds, Brouwer, and van Wolffelaar, 1988). Van Wolffelaar, Brouwer, and Rothengatter (1990) found that there is a disproportionately greater problem for aging adults in divided attention situations and directly linked this to a higher crash rate for aging adults in time-pressured, complex traffic situations.

The minimum required information for traffic management includes: (1) a statement of the problem; and (2) the action statement(s) (i.e., a driver needs to know what to do and one good reason for doing it). Additional elements are included as needed for a specific situation. The key here is not to burden the driver with unnecessary information. Only about two-thirds of drivers are able to recall completely four pieces of information (problem, effect, attention, and action); however, 80–90 percent can recall the action message (Huchingson, Koppa, and Dudek, 1978). Two problems in message presentation must be avoided: (1) providing too much information in too short a time; and (2) providing ambiguous information that leaves either the intent of the message or the desired driver response uncertain.

The first problem does not refer solely to reading time difficulties, as discussed above; instead, it refers to the number of ideas, or "information units," contained in a message. Certainly, the number of words displayed on a sign is important, but so is the manner in which words are grouped. Units containing one word (DELAY), two words (DELAY AHEAD), or many words (MAJOR DELAY AT HIGH STREET) are equally difficult to remember when the display is no longer in sight. However, a series of, say, six units of information in a message displayed on a permanent CMS will be easier to remember if presented in two phases of three units each than if all six units are presented on a single phase. Studies have concluded that no more than three units of information should be displayed on one sequence when all three units must be recalled by drivers (Huchingson et al., 1978; Dudek et al., 1981; Gish, 1995).

Gish (1995) conducted a human factors laboratory study addressing the perceived timeliness, accuracy, and credibility of CMS messages using both younger (ages 16 to 33) and older (ages 65 to 84) test subjects. Results showed that correct recall of the first CMS phase (a downstream speed advisory) was nearly 100 percent for both age groups. However, successive phases of information (containing downstream delay and route diversion information) were recalled less accurately. For the delay information (second phase), correct recall for the younger subjects was about 82 percent, versus 60 percent for the older subjects. For route numbers (third phase), correct recall was 55 percent for the younger subjects and 19 percent for older subjects. These results reinforce the earlier recommendation that a maximum of two phases should be used.

Aging drivers participating in focus groups stated that CMS's with multiple phases were difficult, if not impossible to read. They stated that even when there was enough time to read two phases, once both were read, it was easy to forget what the first phase indicated (Kihl, 2005). Further, these focus group participants indicated that there was no need to offer explanations on the signs; just provide direction to the driver.

When a message must be divided into two phases, it is desirable to repeat key words from the first phase on the second phase, to provide assurance that all drivers see the message at least once. This also allows information rehearsal, as provided by an additional "learning trial," which will facilitate message recall when the device is no longer in sight. A recommended standard practice is therefore to put the problem on line 1, the location on line 2, and alternate either the effect and action or diversion information on line three, repeating lines 1 and 2 on both phases.

The second type of problem can occur when an unfamiliar word or abbreviation is used, when a word is hyphenated or a phrase is divided inappropriately, or when an abbreviation or a word can mean different things in different word pairings or contexts. Ambiguity occurs, for example, when CENTER LANE is used on a freeway with four or more lanes in one direction. Another example is the use of LANE CLOSED versus LANE BLOCKED, to denote a prolonged closure for construction/maintenance versus a temporary blockage due to a crash or stall. To foster the most simple and consistent practice for motorists, LANE CLOSED is recommended under both roadwork and incident conditions, because at the time the message is displayed, the lane is effectively closed. Finally, neither FREEWAY BLOCKED nor FREEWAY CLOSED should ever be used when at least one lane is open to traffic.

Abbreviations also have the potential to be misunderstood by some percentage of drivers, exacerbating message comprehension problems for individuals with (age-related) diminished capabilities. It has been determined that certain abbreviations are understood by at least 85 percent of the driving public independent of the specific context (e.g., BLVD = boulevard). A second category of abbreviations are understood by at least 75 percent of the driving population but only with a prompt word, (e.g., LOC means "local" when shown with "traffic"). Other abbreviations are prone to be frequently confused with another word (e.g., WRNG could mean either "warning" or "wrong") and should be avoided. Following are lists of abbreviations in three categories, extracted from Dudek et al. (1981) and Durkop and Dudek (2001):

  1. those that are acceptable (understood by at least 85 percent of the driving population) when shown alone (Table 63);

  2. those that are not acceptable and, therefore, should not be used (Table 64); and

  3. those that require a prompt word (Table 65). Table 63 also includes abbreviations taken from the MUTCD, as well as common contractions used in the English language.

Most of the abbreviations in these tables have been incorporated into the MUTCD (FHWA, 2009), as Tables 1A-1 to 1A-3 in section 1A.15; Section 6F.60 of the MUTCD states that when abbreviations are used on CMS's, they should be easily understood, and refers the practitioner to section 1A.15.

Table 63. "Acceptable" abbreviations and phrases for frequently used words. Source: Dudek, et al. (1981), Durkop and Dudek (2001).
Word Abbreviation Word Abbreviation
Alternate ALT Left Lane LFT LN
Avenue AVE Major Accident MAJ ACCDT
Boulevard BLVD Maintenance MAINT
Can Not CAN"T Normal NORM
Center CNTR Parking PKING
Center Lane CTR LN Parking Lot PRK LOT
Do Not DON"T Right Lane RGT LN
Emergency EMER Road RD
Emergency Vehicle EMER VEH Service SERV
Entrance, Enter ENT Shoulder SHLDR
Expressway EXPWY Slippery SLIP
Freeway FRWY, FWY Speed SPD
Highway HWY Street ST
Information INFO Traffic TRAF
It Is IT"S Travelers TRVLRS
Junction JCT Warning WARN
Lane Closed LN CLSD Weight Limit WT LIMIT
Left LFT Will Not WON"T
Table 64. Abbreviations and phrases that are "not acceptable." Source: Dudek, et al. (1981); Durkop and Dudek (2001).
Abbreviation Intended Word Common Misinterpretation
ACC Accident Access (Road)
ALT RT Alternate Routes Don't know meaning
CLRS Clears Colors
DLY Delay Daily
EX Exit Don't know meaning
FDR Feeder Federal
FEED RD Feeder Road Feed Road
FRNTG RD Frontage Road Front Road
L Left Lane (Merge)
LT Light (Traffic) Left
MAJ CONG Major Congestion Major Construction
PARK Parking Park
POLL Pollution (Index) Poll
RD WK Road Work Road Walk
RED Reduce Red
STAD Stadium Standard
VIC OF Vicinity of Don't know meaning
WRNG Warning Wrong
Table 65. Abbreviations that are "acceptable with a prompt." Source: Dudek, et al. (1981), Durkop and Dudek (2001).
Word+ Abbreviation Prompt
Access ACCES Road
Ahead AHD Fog*
Blocked BLKD Lane*
Bridge BRDG [Name]*
Condition COND Traffic*
Congested CONG Traffic*
Construction CONST Ahead
Downtown DWNTN Traffic*
Eastbound E-BND Traffic
Exit EXT Next*
Express EXP Lane
Hazardous HAZ Road
Interstate I [Number]
Local LOC Traffic
Major MAJ Accident
Mile MI [Number]*
Minor MNR Accident
Minute(s) MIN [Number]*
Northbound N-BND Traffic
Oversized OVRSZ Load
Prepare PREP To Stop
Pavement PVMT Wet*
Quality QLTY Air*
Roadwork RDWK Ahead [Distance]
Route RTE Best*, Detour*
Southbound S-BND Traffic
Temporary TEMP Route
Township TWNSHP Limits
Turnpike TRNPK [Name]*
Upper, Lower UPR, LWR Level
Vehicle VEH Stalled*
Westbound W-BND Traffic
Cardinal Directions N, E, S, W [Number]

* Prompt word should precede abbreviation.

+ The words and abbreviations shown in normal type are understood by at least 85 percent of the driving population. Those shown in boldface type are understood by at least 75 percent of the driving population, and public education is recommended prior to their usage.

45. Channelization (Path Guidance)

Channelization systems include the use of cones, posts, tubular markers, barricades, panels, drums, amber-flashing and steady-burn lights, and standard and raised/recessed pavement markings. They are used to direct motorists into the open lanes and to guide them through the work area. They must provide a long detection distance and be highly conspicuous under both day and night conditions. Using data collected by the police, it has been estimated that anywhere from 80 to 86 percent of the crashes in work zones can be attributed to driver error (Nemeth and Migletz, 1978; Hargroves and Martin, 1980). Hargroves and Martin (1980) found that crashes with fixed objects within a work zone account for a greater percentage than other crash types, such as rear-end or sideswipe. Nemeth and Migletz (1978) found that nighttime crashes are concentrated in the taper area. Humphreys, Maulden, and Sullivan (1979) identified the most significant problems with channelization in work zones as: (1) failure to use, or hazardous use of, temporary concrete barriers; and (2) inadequate or inconsistent use of devices and methods in closing roadways and establishing lane-closure tapers.

Aging drivers, like alcohol-impaired and fatigued drivers, show reduced sensitivity to contrast. Olson (1988) pointed out that the brightness of a traffic control device is the main factor in its attention-getting capability: in a visually complex environment, the brightness must be increased by a factor of 10 to achieve conspicuity equivalent to that found in a low-complexity environment. A major problem at night is reduction in contrast sensitivity, which makes it difficult to see even large objects when they cannot be distinguished from their background. Aging drivers also have difficulty processing information due to less effective scanning behavior and eye movements, diminished visual field size, difficulty in selective attention, and slower decision making. Inconsistent use of drums and traffic cones to delineate the travel path may be a particular problem for aging drivers, especially when applied in the presence of remnants of old lane markings, because such inconsistency is confusing and aging drivers (and inattentive drivers) are not able to react as quickly to conflicting traffic cues (National Transportation Safety Board, 1992). To compensate for their slower information-processing capabilities, their reduced visual capabilities, and their slower reaction time, aging drivers often drive more slowly. Although driver age was not studied, Hargroves and Martin (1980) found that slow-moving vehicles were overrepresented in work-zone crashes. Aging drivers also show reductions in lane-keeping ability, which is further compromised when they are required to attend to other tasks, in unfamiliar surroundings. Finally, steering abilities may be adversely affected by physical problems such as arthritis.

Table 66. Cross-references of related entries for channelization (path guidance).
Applications in Standard Reference Manuals
MUTCD (2009) NCHRP 500-Volume 9 (2004) Traffic Engineering Handbook (2009)
Sect. 6B.01

Sects. 6C.05, 6C.08, 6D.01, 6F.13, 6F.14, 6F.21 through 6F.24, 6F.32, & 6F.63 & 6F.81

Sects. 6G.04 & 6G.07

Sect. 6G.10 through 6G.18

Figs. 6H-3, 6H-5 through 6H-7, 6H-10 through 6H-12, 6H-15, 6H-18, 6H-21 through 6H-34, 6H-36 through 6H-44, & 6H-46 plus associated notes for each fig.
Pgs. V-26-V-27, Sect. on Strategy 3.1 B11: Improve Traffic Control at Work Zones (T) Pg. 663 Paras. 6-7

McGee and Knapp (1979) performed an analytic study to develop a performance requirement/standard for the detection and recognition of retroreflective devices (cones, drums, panels, and barricades) used in work zones. The performance standard developed in this study, presented in terms of visibility requirements (i.e., the distance at which motorists should be able to detect and recognize the devices at night) and established using the principles of driver information needs and the requirement for decision sight distance, calls for a minimum visibility distance of 900 ft when illuminated by the low beams of standard automobile headlights at night under normal atmospheric conditions.

Pain, McGee, and Knapp (1981) evaluated the effectiveness of traffic cones and tubular markers, vertical panels, drums, barricades, and steady-burn lights in laboratory studies, in controlled field studies, and at actual construction sites. Two-hundred fifty-four subjects between the ages of 17 and 60+ participated; over half of the subjects were between ages 21 and 40, and 7 percent of the subjects were age 60 or older. Overall, there were no major differences between the device categories in the daytime. At night, barricades, panels, drums, cones, and tubular markers were also equivalent when the optimized cone and tubular marker retroreflectorization was used (two bands of retroreflective material for cones and one band for tubular markers totaling 150 to 200 in2, or roughly the amount provided by a 12- to 14-in collar) of retroreflective material with SIA of at least 250. However, tubular markers and cones with 6 in of collar resulted in diminished nighttime performance. The variables manipulated in the cone optimization study included amount of retroreflectorization (69, 138, 207, 276, and 345 in2), [corresponding to single bands measuring 6-, 10-, 14-, 17-, and 20-in wide; number of bands of retroreflective material (1, 2, or 3); 3 types of retroreflectorization plus 1 internally illuminated cone (polycarbonate Reflexite with SIA of 2000 at entrance angle -4° and observation angle 0.1°, high intensity with SIA of 300 at entrance angle -4° and observation angle 0.1°, and polycarbonate Reflexite plus vinyl Reflexite); color of retroreflectorization (white and yellow), 3 sizes (18-, 28-, and 36- in tall); 3 device spacings (half, regular, and double-speed limit). The variables manipulated in the tubular marker study included amount of retroreflectorization (14, 28, 43, 57, and 71 percent of area covered, corresponding to bands measuring 150-, 300-, 450-, 600-, and 950-mm wide); number of bands (1, 2, 4, 6, or 8); the same retroreflectorization levels and colors as for the cone study, 3 sizes (18-, 28-, and 42-in tall); and the same device spacings as described for the cone study.

In comparing the meaning of chevrons versus stripes, Pain et al. (1981) found that diagonal, horizontal, and vertical stripes conveyed no consistent directional information; chevrons, though less easily detected than the stripe patterns, effectively and unambiguously indicated that a movement to the left or right was required. Since diagonal, horizontal, and vertical stripes conveyed no consistent direction information; Pain et al. (1981) concluded that there was no reason to have a diagonal stripe pattern for left and right 'sidedness." They pointed out, however, that only one direction of diagonal should be allowed in an array so there is always a consistent pattern or image on devices.

In terms of device spacing, comparisons of regular speed-limit spacing (55 ft in the test), half-spacing (27.5 ft), and double-spacing (110 ft) of Type I barricades and 8-in x 24-in) panels showed that changes in spacing produced little impact on driver behavior. There was no significant speed or lateral placement differences between half, regular, and double speed-limit spacing during the day. At night, however, when devices were placed at half-spacing, they produced a speed reduction, apparently from the illusion that the motorist was going faster than he or she actually was. Devices placed at double-spacing tended not to perform as well as when they were placed at regular speed-limit spacing, as drivers made lane changes and detected arrays of traffic control devices sooner with shorter spacing. From these findings, Pain et al. (1981) recommended that: (1) all devices be placed at speed limit spacing for most conditions and, in all cases, along the taper or transition section; (2) if there is no construction work or hazard in the closed lane for a substantial length, or traffic delays, the spacing can be increased to no more than twice the speed limit; and (3) shorter spacing may prove to be useful where speed reduction is desired.

Device-specific findings by Pain et al. (1981) are as follows:

Other findings were reported for comparisons of steady-burn lights and Type II and Type III sheeting. The steady-burn lights provided the longest detection distances at night compared with all other materials, and they more than tripled the distance (or zone) in which lane changing occurred before the taper. In comparisons of Type II sheeting and Type III sheeting on cone and tubular marker optimization tests, Type III was significantly better at night on a flat road. Narrow-angle sheeting, even though offering high brightness, was not effective under certain sight geometry characteristics, such as hills and curves. Type III sheeting and steady-burn lights were comparable in terms of point-of-lane-change and array detection distance; however, the authors noted that the effect of vertical or horizontal curvature must be considered.

There have been mixed results regarding the effectiveness of steady-burn lights in highway work zones. The use of steady-burn lights mounted on channelizing devices has been shown to significantly influence driver behavior in some work-zone configurations, and they are particularly effective in left-lane closures (KLD Associates, 1992). Although drivers age 55 and older consistently showed poorer performance than younger drivers in all study conditions, evidence was found that the use of lights improved the performance of aging test subjects. The variables manipulated in this study included work-zone configuration (left-lane, right-lane, and shoulder closures), device type (panels versus drums), and light placement (every device, alternate devices, no lights). Drivers of all ages were able to identify lane and shoulder closures from greater distances when lights were used on channelization devices, as opposed to when the channelizing devices were used alone. Steady-burn lights produced a higher percentage of correct responses (determining the direction the channelizing devices were leading) for all driver age groups when used in left-lane closures than in right-lane closures. Interestingly, the use of lights on every other drum or vertical panel (placement on alternate devices) generated more correct responses than the use of lights on consecutive devices. More generally, the literature suggests that in environments characterized by high-speed operations, compromised visibility due to inclement weather, and/or complex maneuvers required as a result of work-zone configuration, the deployment of steady-burn lights should be considered on all channelizing devices used for right-lane closures.

However, Pant, Huang, and Krishnamurthy (1992) obtained a different result when they examined the lane-changing behavior of motorists in advance of tapered sections as they drove an instrumented vehicle through work zones during the day, at night when steady-burn lights were placed on drums, and at night when the steady-burn lights were removed. They measured the traffic volume at several locations in each lane in advance of the taper. Results showed that the steady-burn lights had little effect on driver behavior in the work zones studied. It was concluded by Pant et al. that steady-burn lights have little value in work zones that employ drums with high intensity sheeting and a flashing arrow panel as channelizing devices.

Opiela and Knoblauch (1990) conducted laboratory and field studies to determine the optimal spacing and use of devices for channelization purposes in the taper or tangent sections of work zones. In the laboratory study, the recognition distances of eight different device types, spaced at the standard distance and at 1.5 and 2.0 times the standard distance, were measured for 240 subjects. Results indicated variability between the performance of most channelizing devices across the spacings tested. Right- and left-lane closures were then used at six actual work zones, to test the various device spacings under both day and night conditions. Field data were collected at four points equally spaced over 2,000 ft before the work zone and the activity at the start of the taper for the lane closure, according to the premise that the most effective treatment would minimize the percentage of traffic in the closed lane at the start of the taper. Statistical analysis of 2,100 observation periods lasting 5 minutes each showed that neither type of device (round drums, oblong drums, Type II barricades, and cones with retroreflective collars) nor device spacings (55, 80, and 110 ft) had a significant effect on driver lane-changing behavior.

Cottrell (1981) also found that driver lane-change response was not strongly dependent on the channelizing device employed in a work-zone taper. The objective of this study was to evaluate the effectiveness of alternative orange-and-white chevron patterns on vertical panels and barricades that form an arrow pointing in the direction in which traffic is being diverted, compared with traffic cones, simulated drum vertical panels, and Type II barricades and vertical panels with standard orange-and-white striping patterns. The measure of effectiveness was the position of lane changing relative to the transition taper. Although the subjective evaluation revealed that chevron patterns were preferred over the presently used patterns because of their clear directional message, the positions of lane changing were similar for the stripes and chevrons. With respect to the spacing of devices, it was generally found that lane changes occurred more frequently at greater distances from the taper when the devices were spaced every 40 ft, as opposed to every 80 ft.

In a supplemental test, the effectiveness of the concrete safety-shaped barrier (CSSB), also referred to as a "Jersey" barrier in some jurisdictions, was compared with that of the channelizing devices studied (Cottrell, 1981). The barrier was marked with steady-burn warning lights and 6-in reflectors and had a slope of 16:1 for the 192-ft taper. The CSSB was rated equal to the cone during the daytime and lower than all other devices based on the lane-change positions. It was recommended that a supplemental taper of channelization devices be used with the CSSB. In a study of concrete barrier visibility, Pain et al. (1981) found that retroreflectors were superior to retroreflectorized tape. Logically, the most conspicuous types of retroreflective devices, such as those containing cube-corner lenses, will be potentially the most effective in this regard.

Overall, Pain et al. (1981) concluded that most devices show relatively successful detection and path guidance performance. However, a major deterrent to effectiveness is not the device itself; instead, poor positioning, dirt, and overturned devices destroy the visual line or path created by the channelizing devices. Therefore, although use of appropriate devices is important, of equal importance is conscientious set-up and care of channelizing devices used in the work zones.

In consideration of the threat posed to drivers by passenger compartment intrusion or interference with vehicle control, or the threat to workers and other traffic from impact debris, plastic drums, cones, tubular markers, and vertical panels used as channelizing devices presented no hazards in full-scale vehicle crash tests (Bryden, 1990). However, Types I and II barricades and portable signs and supports formed impact debris, which was often thrown long distances through work zones, posing a threat to workers and other traffic. The American Traffic Safety Services Association (ATSSA) is opposed to the use of metal drums in work zones as channelizing devices, as they pose a hazard to motorists as well as workers in the zone (TranSafety, 1987). They suggest the use of plastic drums, which are safer. Riedel (1986) described studies showing that a substantial number of work-zone crashes occur in the taper and the crossover where channelization devices are located. The frequency of crashes involving drums has led to the use of forgiving devices such as plastic drums, which in tests have been shown to be safer than steel drums. Juergens (1972) noted that because barricades are inherently fixed-object hazards, they should not be used as primary delineation to guide traffic. Further, they should not be used unless the construction hazard the motorist may encounter is greater than the hazard of striking the barricades. A concern with the use of steady-burn lights mounted on channelizing devices was highlighted in full-scale vehicle crash tests evaluating the performance of work-zone traffic control devices, where warning lights attached to these devices were thrown free, posing a potential threat to workers and other traffic (Bryden, 1990).

46. Delineation of Crossovers/Alternate Travel Paths

Studies have established that: (1) a substantial proportion of construction work-zone crashes occur in the taper and the crossover, where channelizing devices are usually located; (2) darkness is associated with an increase in the frequency of crashes in these areas; and (3) construction zones are associated with increases in the incidence of fixed-object, rear-end, and head-on crashes (Graham, Paulsen, and Glennon, 1977). Nemeth and Rathi (1983), studying crash types in construction zones on the Ohio Turnpike, found that 52.4 percent of the crashes were with fixed objects, and 68.3 percent of the crossover crashes involved collisions with channelizing devices or other objects. In this study, 69.4 percent of the crashes at the first curve of a crossover occurred at night. Nemeth and Migletz (1978) found that 60.7 percent of single-vehicle fixed-object crashes were collisions with drums and 29.8 percent of all crashes involved collisions with drums. They also found that the proportion of crashes involving construction objects (drums) at night is significantly higher than the proportion of daylight construction object crashes. The results of these studies highlight the need for highly conspicuous and properly installed and maintained channelizing devices.

Table 67. Cross-references of related entries for delineation of crossovers/alternative travel paths.
Applications in Standard Reference Manuals
MUTCD (2009) AASHTO Green Book (2011)
Sects. 6F.80 through 6F.83

Sects. 6F.76, 6F.84, 6F.85

Sects. 6F.69, 6F.70, & 6F.79

Sect. 6G.04

Sect. 6G.11

Sect. 6G.12

Sect. 6G.14

Figs. 6H-7, 6H-9, 6H-19, 6H-20, 6H-39 through 6H-41, & 6H-45 plus associated notes for each fig.

Ch. 6I
Pgs. V-26-V-27, Sect. on Strategy 3.1 B11: Improve Traffic Control at Work Zones (T)

The relationships between functional capabilities of aging drivers and their performance that are likely to be of greatest operational significance as they approach and negotiate a crossover in a work zone can be summarized as follows. Age-related declines in acuity (both static and dynamic) and contrast sensitivity will delay recognition of channelizing devices and pavement markings and will delay comprehension of the information provided by advance warning signs. This information loss in the early stages of the driver's vehicle control task will be compounded by attentional and decision making deficits shown to increase with increasing age, with age differences in performance magnified as serial processing demands for conflict avoidance and compliance with traffic control messages increase during the approach to the work zone. Age-related decrements in the "useful field of view," selective attention, and divided attention, and attention-switching capabilities will slow the initiation of a driver's response when a lane change is required prior to the transition zone, or maneuvering through channelization across the median. In addition, less efficient working memory processes may translate into riskier operations for aging drivers in unfamiliar areas if concurrent search for and recognition of navigational cues is required, as such demands disproportionately tax 'spare capacity" for lanekeeping and conflict avoidance for aging operators. Finally, the execution of vehicle-turning movements becomes more difficult for aging drivers as bone and muscle mass decrease, joint flexibility is lost, and range of motion diminishes. Simple reaction time, while not significantly slower for aging drivers responding to expected stimuli under nominal operating conditions, suffers operationally significant decrements with each additional response to an unexpected stimulus, e.g., as required in emergency situations. In addition, aging drivers' increased sensitivity to glare and reduced dark adaptation ability will compound the difficulties described above while driving at night.

The National Transportation Safety Board (NTSB) has expressed concern about the lack of positive separation of opposing traffic in work zones (NTSB, 1992). The NTSB uses "positive barrier," or "positive separation of traffic," to refer to the use of concrete barriers to separate traffic. (A number of States distinguish between these terms, using "positive separation" to describe various channelization treatments which do not necessarily involve use of a physical concrete barrier.) The NTSB (1992) emphasizes that, "Fatal crash rates increase significantly when an interstate highway is switched from a four-lane, divided operation to a two-lane, two-way operation (TLTWO) during construction work." Research on the use of channelization and barrier delineation for TLTWO's is described below.

A crossover requires a change in direction and may require a reduction in speed. This requires adequate advance warning of the lane and speed reduction, conspicuous and unambiguous delineation/channelization in the transition zone, and conspicuous separation of opposing traffic the length of the TLTWO. One survey of drivers in Houston and Dallas, Texas by Hawkins, Kacir, and Ogden (1992) found that only half of the respondents correctly understood that they should turn before reaching the CROSSOVER sign (D13-1) when this device was shown in a field placement in an arterial work zone. Of course, the D13-1 sign panel is identified in the MUTCD as a device used in permanent installations on divided highways, not as a temporary device for use in construction zones. The poor comprehension of motorists for such an explicit message is alarming, nevertheless, and suggests the need for heightened conspicuity of guidance information in this situation. Hawkins et al. recommended that the spacing of channelizing devices be decreased in the vicinity of a crossover to reduce drivers' confusion.

Next, Pang and Yu (1981) conducted a study to verify whether concrete barriers were justified at transition zones adjacent to TLTWO's on normally divided highways, based on crash experience in several construction zone TLTWO's. They found that 34 of the 44 total crashes that occurred in TLTWO's were within the transition zone. Four head-on crashes occurred on two-way, two-lane segments away from the transitions. The transition zone was defined as the roadway section at which traffic flow was converted from a four- to a two-lane operation and vice versa. The absence of opposing traffic precluded the occurrence of head-on crashes during the study period; however, more than one-half of the crashes (56 percent) had the potential of becoming head-on collisions. The authors concluded that on relatively low-volume highways, delineation devices appear to be adequate at transition zones, assuming they are placed properly. A regression analysis provided by Pang (1979) indicated that as annual average daily traffic increases, the crash rate at transition zones also increases, with a concurrent increase in the head-on crash rate at the transition zone.

Project duration and approach speed are two other variables that appear to affect the head-on crash rate at transitions (Pang and Yu, 1981). Graham (1977) concluded that as project duration increases, the crash rate at the transitions decreases. Expectancy issues were highlighted as a plausible explanation. Pang and Yu (1981) reported that because the crash rate in the transition zone increases with shorter project duration, concrete barriers may be necessary for short-term projects. However, long-term projects are expected to have a greater number of crashes owing to a longer period of exposure. Thus, installation of concrete barriers would be more economically justified for long-term projects than for short-term ones. With regard to approach speed, it can be expected that as speed to the transition increases, the chances of a head-on collision would also increase, due to the tendency of vehicles to stray out of their lanes at curves such as those present in transition zones. Pang and Yu (1981) suggested that concrete barriers appear to be justified at transition zones where approach speeds are high.

The conspicuity of concrete safety shaped barriers (CSSB's) is an important issue. Their composition provides little contrast with the roadway pavement, making them difficult to see at night, particularly in the rain, and under opposing headlight glare conditions. Proper barrier delineation treatments will provide drivers with a defined path during darkness and adverse weather conditions. Standard barrier delineation treatments include Type C steady-burn warning lights on top of the barrier, retroreflective devices on the top or side of the barrier, vertical panels placed on top of the temporary concrete barrier, and retroreflective pavement markings on the side of the barrier. The results of studies of barrier delineation in work zones have been mixed (Ullman and Dudek, 1988). For instance, Mullowney (1978) suggested that delineation should be mounted on the top of the barrier so it will retain its reflectivity longer and require less maintenance. However, Ogwoaba (1986) recommended side-mounted concrete barrier delineation so that the delineators are not masked by oncoming headlight glare. The size and brightness of delineators is another controversial topic, with some studies suggesting the use of larger but less bright devices (Davis, 1983; Bracket, et al., 1984; Kahn, 1985) and others recommending smaller, brighter reflectors (Mullowney, 1978; Ogwoaba, 1986). Kahn (1985) found that the delineation of portable concrete barriers improved considerably through the use of cylindrical reflectors on top and smaller units on the side of the barrier at 25-ft intervals. Delineator spacings ranging from 25 ft to 200 ft (7.6 m to 61 m) have been recommended by various studies.

Ullman and Dudek (1988) conducted a study of five barrier delineation treatments, using observations of driver performance to determine how different delineator types, spacings, and mounting positions on the barrier affect nighttime traffic operating in the travel lane next to the barrier. An additional objective of the study was to determine how the visibility and brightness of different types of delineators deteriorate over time because of dirt and road film; in a controlled field study, drivers ages 18 to 56 were asked to provide subjective evaluations of delineator brightness. The study was not conducted at a work zone, but was conducted on an illuminated urban freeway with four lanes in each direction. The CSSB was located 1 ft from the inside travel lane. The five delineation treatments were: (1) top-mounted cube-corner lenses at 200-ft spacing; (2) side-mounted cube-corner lenses at 50-ft spacing; (3) top-mounted retroretroreflective brackets at 50-ft spacing; (4) side-mounted retroreflective brackets at 200-ft spacing; and (5) top-mounted retroreflective cylinders at 50-ft spacing. The cube-corner reflector (treatments 1 and 2) had a diameter of 3.25 in. The brackets (treatments 3 and 4) were 3 in (75 mm) high and 4.25 in wide, and were covered with high intensity sheeting. The cylindrical tube (treatment 5) had a diameter of 3 in and was 6 in high, and was wrapped with high intensity sheeting. Before-and-after data were obtained for the following measures of effectiveness: lane distribution, lane straddling, and lateral distance from the left rear tire to the bottom of the CSSB.

Results of the driver performance data collected by Ullman and Dudek (1988) showed that the treatments had very little practical effect on lane distribution. Lane-straddling rates at all of the treatment segments were low during the higher volume nighttime hours; however, a significant increase in lane straddling occurred for Treatment 2. The data suggested that the combination of close delineator spacing and the side-mounted position may make some drivers too apprehensive of driving near the barrier. Lateral distance data showed significant differences during the higher volume nighttime hours for Treatment 4 and Treatment 5. Lateral distance distributions shifted away from the barrier at Treatment 4 and closer to the barrier at Treatment 5. Subjective evaluations for clean delineators showed brightness rankings to be the same for all treatments. Treatments 1–4 received adequate ratings from at least 80 percent of the subjects, while Treatment 5 was rated adequate by only 50 percent of the subjects. With respect to each treatment's relative effectiveness in helping drivers maintain a safe travel path next to the CSSB, the rankings did not differ significantly; however, Treatment 5 again received the worst score. Subjects stated that side-mounted delineators were preferable to top-mounted delineators because side-mounted delineation provided a more direct line of sight, a better indication of the location of the wall, and a more realistic perception of the lane width. For dirt-covered delineators, Treatment 2 was rated as brightest and most effective, while Treatment 5 was rated as dimmest and least effective. Although further research was deemed necessary, the study authors recommended the use of cube-corner lenses for delineating CSSB's in narrow freeway median applications, because these delineators do not lose their reflectivity due to dirt and grime as quickly as those covered with high intensity sheeting. In addition, for situations with limited lateral clearance, as is common with TLTWO's, top-mounted delineation is recommended, because side-mounted close delineator spacing results in lane straddling if the barrier is located close to the travel lanes. Although subjects indicated a preference for close spacings, driver performance data did not show any differences between 50-ft and 200-ft spacing. The authors recommended that a 200-ft spacing be considered maximum, and that closer spacings may be necessary for CSSB's on sharp curves. The treatments were also deemed appropriate for CSSB's in work zones.

On divided highways with narrow medians, which are often created when barriers are used in crossover situations in work zones, drivers are subject to blinding glare from opposing vehicle headlights. This is particularly problematic for aging drivers who have a reduction in their dark adaptation ability and increased sensitivity to glare. This results in reduced visibility of roadway alignment and channelization, and increases the possibility of crashes. Glare screens can solve the problem, as well as reduce rubbernecking and its associated problems. The Pennsylvania Department of Transportation discontinued the use of the standard glare-control mesh screen in 1976, based on maintenance difficulties, and has employed a paddle-type system in its place (Maurer, 1984). The system consists of plastic airfoil-shaped paddles, which when mounted resembles a picket fence. Results of a 5-year study have shown that the paddle-type system reduces headlight glare satisfactorily and is more cost-effective, both in terms of installation and maintenance, than metal mesh screen. The system was also found to be beneficial as a temporary control for channelizing traffic around a construction work zone, when screening was placed at the transition or the taper zone at the ends of the work zone (Maurer, 1984). Kelly and Bryden (1983) reported that a glare screen consisting of individual plastic louvers 36 in high, mounted vertically on a guiderail or median barrier spaced at 24-in centers, performed as expected in two safety improvement projects.

47. Temporary Pavement Markings

Preconstruction centerlines and edge lines that are not obliterated may confuse drivers about the exact locations of lanes. The National Transportation Safety Board (1992) has reported that although guidelines exist for proper signing and striping in construction areas, the traffic control techniques used in many jurisdictions are not in compliance with the guidelines. Lewis (1985) stated that if drivers are presented with conflicting information (as may be the case in a work zone), they will generally choose to follow the pavement, as the pavement itself is a primary source of information for drivers. This points to a need for unambiguous pavement delineation patterns in work zones, to provide clear guidance—particularly at night and under adverse weather conditions—and to accommodate drivers with visual limitations such as those associated with normal aging.

The research findings that have the greatest bearing on age differences in drivers' ability to acquire and use information provided by roadway delineation are a decline in spatial contrast sensitivity and acuity for aging drivers, and a general slowing of responses because of deficits in visual search ability that slows discrimination of more important from less important information in a driving scene.

Discrimination of the boundaries of the traveled way often involves only slight differences in the brightness of the road surface versus the shoulder or surrounding land. The ability to obtain such "edge information" depends upon a driver's sensitivity to contrast. Age differences in contrast sensitivity, beginning at approximately age 40 and becoming progressively more exaggerated with advancing age, demonstrate significant decrements in performance for aging persons (Owsley, Sekuler, and Siemsen, 1983). Under constant viewing conditions, aging observers have lower contrast sensitivity especially in situations where there is a reduction in ambient light levels. A 60-year-old driver requires 2.5 times the contrast needed by a 23-year-old driver (Blackwell and Blackwell, 1971).

Age decrements in visual search and scanning capabilities are widely reported in gerontological research. Rackoff and Mourant (1979) measured visual search patterns for 10 young (ages 21–29) and 13 older (ages 60–70) subjects as they drove on a freeway under day and night conditions in low to moderate traffic. They reported that differences between young and older test subjects' performance were most apparent at night, and that older subjects required more time to acquire the minimum information needed for vehicle control. Thus, older drivers require delineation information that is optimal from the standpoints of both attention conspicuity and search conspicuity downstream, and that provides unambiguous path guidance cues for moment-to-moment steering control. Uncertainty about roadway heading and lane position has been cited by older driver focus group members as reasons for driving slower, for erratic maneuvers caused by last-second steering corrections, and for simply avoiding nighttime operations (Staplin, Lococo, and Sim, 1990). An exaggeration of the difficulties older drivers have in rapidly discerning the correct travel path may be expected in construction zones, where drivers must respond to temporary pavement markings that are often in competition with preexisting stripes and/or misleading informal cues provided by variation in the surface characteristics of the road, shoulder, or median.

Table 68. Cross-references of related entries for temporary pavement markings.
Applications in Standard Reference Manuals
MUTCD (2009) NCHRP 500-Volume 9 (2004) Traffic Engineering Handbook (2009)
Sects. 6F.63, 6F.64, 6F.65

Sects. 6F.77, 6F.78, 6F.79

Sect. 6G.06

Figs. 6H-7, 6H-12, 6H-14, 6H-24, 6H-29, 6H-32 through 6H-34, 6H-36, 6H-38 through 6H-42, & 6H-44 plus associated notes for each fig.
Pgs. V-26-V-27, Sect. on Strategy 3.1 B11: Improve Traffic Control at Work Zones (T) Pg. 669, Sect. on Pavement Markings

These diminished capabilities must be considered in relation to specific information needs, when negotiating work zones, while also taking into account the time (distance) in which these needs must be satisfied. The information needs may be loosely contrasted according to the discrimination of continuous versus discrete roadway features (i.e., the perception and recognition of the boundaries of the traveled way, as opposed to a singular location which must be avoided (e.g., an island, barrier, or abutment) or where a path selection decision must be acted upon (e.g., a ramp gore, pavement width transition point, or intersection)). Furthermore, delineation must provide information to a driver permitting roadway feature recognition both at "long" preview distances up to and sometimes exceeding 5 s travel time, and at the more immediate proximities (i.e., within 1 s travel time) where attention is directed for instant-to-instant vehicle control responses.

An investigation of age-related differences in the required contrast for pavement delineation showed that an aging driver (ages 65–80) test sample required a level of contrast 20–30 percent higher than a young/middle-aged (ages 19–49) comparison group (Staplin et al., 1990). The differences became exaggerated with glare as an independent variable. An inevitable consequence of these age differences is an increased reliance on delineation elements for path guidance by aging drivers under nighttime conditions, especially against oncoming glare. The "long preview" and the instant-to-instant steering control cues provided by pavement markings are critical to aging drivers under these circumstances.

Raised pavement markers (RPMs) used for delineation of the centerline and edge lines in construction zones have been found to provide improved wet weather and nighttime reflectivity, and are particularly useful when lanes are diverted from their original path (Spencer, 1978). Davis (1983) reported that, compared with conventional pavement markings (e.g., paint), day-night/wet-night visible RPMs improved construction zone traffic performance significantly. In this study, the markers were associated with decreased lane-change frequency and night lane encroachments. In before-and-after comparisons of crash frequencies in two construction projects, the number of crashes and fatalities decreased as a function of RPM installation (Niessner, 1978). In a study investigating vehicle guidance through work zones, Shepard (1989) recommended that closely spaced RPMs should be used as a supplement to existing pavement striping in areas where the roadway alignment changes.

Dudek, Huchingson, and Woods (1986) conducted a study on a test track to examine the effectiveness of temporary pavement markings for use in work zones. Ten candidate treatments were tested during the day, and the most effective treatments were examined at night. All treatments were tested only under dry weather/dry road conditions. The candidate treatments are presented in Table 69 and included patterns with stripes, RPM's, and combinations of stripes and RPM's. Treatment 1 was the control condition in the study.

Table 69. Temporary pavement marking treatments evaluated by Dudek, Huchingson, and Woods (1986).
Treatment Description
1* 4-ft stripes (4 in wide) with 36-ft gaps (control condition)
2* 2-ft stripes (4 in wide) with 38-ft gaps
3* 8-ft stripes (4 in wide) with 32-ft gaps
4* 2-ft stripes (4 in wide) with 18-ft gaps
5* Four nonreflective RPM's at 3-1/3-ft intervals with 30-ft gaps and one retroreflective marker centered in alternate gaps at 80-ft intervals
6* Three nonretroreflective and one retroreflective RPM at 3-1/3-ft intervals with 30-ft gaps
7 2-ft stripes (4 in wide) with 48-ft gaps
8 Treatment 2 plus RPM's at 80-ft intervals
9* Two nonretroreflective RPM's at 4-ft intervals with 36-ft gaps plus one retroreflective RPM centered in each 36-ft gap
10 1-ft stripes (4 in wide) with 19-ft gaps

* Treatments evaluated both day and night

Results of both daylight and nighttime testing indicated that there were no practical differences between treatments when comparing measures of effectiveness developed from speed and distance measurements. Practical differences were arbitrarily defined as at least 4 mph for speed measures and 1 ft for distance measures. The greatest number of erratic maneuvers during daylight occurred for treatments 7 and 8, which consisted of 2-ft stripes and long gaps. Drivers referred to 2-ft stripes as dots. The subjective data indicated that Treatments 5, 6, and 9 were preferred, under both daylight and nighttime conditions. Reasons given were that RPMs clearly identify curves, are highly visible at a great distance, provide noise and vibration when drivers cross them, and stand out more than tape markings. Of the treatments without RPMs, Treatment 3 was the drivers' choice, for both lighting conditions, while Treatment 2 was rated as least effective.

It should be noted that for temporary pavement markings, the MUTCD specifies in section 6F.78 that the same cycle length as permanent markings be used (30 ft), with markings at least 2 ft long, and that half-cycle lengths with a minimum of 2-ft stripes may be used for roadways with severe curvature.

Because subjects tend to perform best when in a controlled, test-track setting and because the range of performance measures are not always sensitive enough to discern small differences between candidate treatments, Dudek, et al. (1988) conducted field studies to compare the safety and operational effectiveness of 1-ft, 2-ft, and 4-ft temporary broken line pavement markings on 40-ft centers in work zones. The study was conducted at night on rural two-lane, two-way highways with 2.0-degree horizontal curvatures, level to rolling terrain, and average speeds between 50 mph and 62 mph. In terms of speed, lateral distance, encroachment, erratic maneuver, and speed profile data for the sample of vehicles with headways of 4 s or more, there were no differences in driver performance between the 1-ft, 2-ft, and 4-ft striping patterns. Analysis of subjective evaluations of the effectiveness of the markings found that the 1-ft stripe was generally rated as poorest, but its mean ranking was not significantly different from that of the 2-ft and 4-ft stripes. Drivers generally preferred the longer stripes, but there was no evidence that the 2-ft or 4-ft stripes were superior to the 1-ft stripe.

In a discussion of the conditions present during this research, Ward (1988) stated that all sites had 12-ft lanes with 4-ft to 10-ft shoulders, the marking material was highly retroreflective yellow tape laid over very black new pavement overlays, and there were no edge lines; therefore, the drivers' focus was a "brilliant ribbon of yellow to follow," resulting in no difference in driver performance between the three stripe lengths. Most important was that none of the treatments were judged as extremely effective, although the 1-ft stripe was rated as poorest, and there was a slight preference for the 4-ft lengths. This is consistent with results obtained by Dudek et al. (1986), where subjects rated 8-ft stripes with 32-ft gaps as the best striping treatment (when RPM's were not available). In the Dudek et al. (1986) study, drivers preferred the treatments with longer stripes, shorter gaps, and RPM's. Hence, the results of the Dudek et al. (1988) study may be applicable only to pavement overlay projects on two-lane, two-way rural roadways, and may not translate to other highway work-zone situations.

Harkey, Mera, and Byington (1992) conducted a study to determine the effects of short-term (interim) pavement markings on driver performance under day, night, wet, and dry weather conditions. The three marking patterns tested included: (1) 2-ft stripes with 38-ft gaps and no edge lines; (2) 4-ft stripes with 36-ft gaps and no edge lines; and (3) 10-ft stripes with 30-ft gaps and edge lines. The measures of effectiveness included lateral placement of the vehicle on the roadway, vehicle speed, number of edge line and lane line encroachments, and number of erratic maneuvers (e.g., sudden speed or directional changes and brake applications). For each operational measure, the 10-ft markings resulted in better driver performance than either the 2-ft or 4-ft temporary marking patterns. Drivers traveled 0.76 mph slower on segments with 4-ft markings and 2.02 mph slower on segments marked with 2-ft markings than on segments marked with 10-ft stripes and edge lines. In addition, compared with the 10-ft pattern, drivers encroached over the lane or edge line 66 percent more often in the presence of the 4-ft temporary marking and 139 percent more often in the presence of the 2-ft markings. These values increased dramatically under night and wet-weather conditions. Comparisons of driver performance between the 4-ft and 2-ft markings showed the following: (1) the speed at which drivers traveled decreased as the length of the lane line decreased; (2) drivers positioned their vehicles closer to the center of the lane as the length of the line increased; (3) the variability of vehicle placement within the lane increased as the length of the lane line decreased; (4) the number of encroachments increased as the length of the lane line decreased; and (5) all operational measures were negatively affected by adverse weather conditions. Results provided evidence of significant decreases in driver performance associated with both the 2-ft and the 4-ft markings, but drivers performed better with the 4-ft stripes compared to the 2-ft stripes. The results suggested that while it may not be practical to place full markings (10-ft segments with 30-ft gaps as specified by MUTCD Section 3A.06) on a temporary basis, measures should be taken to prevent reductions in driver performance which result in increased crash potential. Such measures include the use of longer temporary markings, the addition of RPM's for improved performance under adverse weather conditions, and the appropriate use of warning signs to indicate a change in the pavement marking pattern.

Promising Practices
48. Increased Letter Height for Temporary Work Zone Signs

Description of Practice: Studies have shown that 25-30 percent of crashes within a work zone happen in the advance warning area or transition area. Most of them are rear-end or side-swipe types of crashes. Some of them are preventable with more perception/reaction time. A Caltrans sign comparison study concluded that for aging drivers, early recognition of the proposed sign could result in an additional 1.5 seconds of response time (Caltrans 2012).

Work zone warning signs notify road users of specific situations or conditions on or adjacent to a roadway that might not otherwise be apparent. A solution is needed to improve sign legibility without increasing the cost of providing temporary traffic control. To improve and enhance roadway safety in and around work zones, the largest warning signs practical should be used. However, due to the limitations of the most commonly available portable sign stands, the work zone warning signs are limited to a maximum of 48 in by 48 in. If larger sign sizes are used, they will require larger portable sign stands resulting in a significant increase in cost to provide traffic control in and around work zones.

The California MUTCD recommends the use of larger-than-standard sign, symbol and legend sizes where roadway or road user conditions require greater emphasis. In order to increase sign legibility for TTC zone warning signs, a suggested solution was to identify signs that can have larger legends displayed on some "action" words without increasing the overall sign size maximum of 48 in by 48 in. The proposal calls for increasing the number patch from 8-in to 10-in numbers, and the words "LEFT", "RIGHT", and "CLOSED" from 6-in to 8-in letters. (See Figure 95.)

Figure 95. An image of two warning signs that read 'RIGHT LANE CLOSED AHEAD'.  The sign on the left is yellow sign with black letters, and all letters are the same height.  The sign on the right is an orange work zone sign with black letters.  The words 'RIGHT' and 'CLOSED' have a larger letter height than the other words.

Figure 95. Current (left) and proposed modified (right) temporary work zone sign in California (Caltrans 2012)

Anticipated Benefits to Aging Road Users: As with signs in other driving environments, larger legend size would increase sign legibility and improve work zone safety. Initial results of a Caltrans study indicate that drivers were able to read the modified signs from approximately 100 ft further away. While benefitting all road users, it may especially help aging road users as the larger legend size of action words will gain quicker comprehension of the sign message and from a longer distance, increasing their decision making and response time to the intended action required by the sign.

49. Work Zone Road Safety Audits

Description of Practice: Work zones have increased potential for violating a driver's expectations, which can be especially hazardous for the aging population because of their diminished visual acuity. According to the Fatality Analysis Reporting System (FARS), 609 people were killed in motor vehicle crashes in work zones in 2012. Of those 609 fatalities, 45 were age 65 and older. A Work Zone Road Safety Audit (WZRSA) is a tool agencies can use to help reduce fatalities and serious injury crashes in work zones. It is the formal safety performance evaluation of a work zone by an independent, multidisciplinary team. It detects and reports on potential road safety issues and identifies opportunities to improve the safety of workers and all roadway users, including the aging population.

A Work Zone RSA follows the eight-step process of an RSA combined with characteristics of a typical work zone inspection or process review.

One of the key features of a WZRSA is that it is performed by a multidisciplinary team focused on safety issues. Team members with various backgrounds and experiences can identify issues that may otherwise be overlooked. WZRSA teams may review the potential for safety enhancements to roadway and work zone elements, human factor considerations, enforcement and emergency services issues, and facility operations, whether they currently exist or are planned.

Anticipated Benefits to Aging Road Users: Recommendations from a WZRSA can potentially affect roadway users and workers immediately, improve the safety of the work zone being audited, and improve an agency's overall work zone development and deployment process. The Work Zone Road Safety Audit Guidelines and Prompt Lists is a publication that includes guidance on how to conduct an RSA at all phases of work zone planning, design and deployment. The document explains the importance of the Work Zone RSA and navigates practitioners through the RSA process. Executing WZRSAs can make a work zone safer, mitigate the potential for risk claims, reduce the societal cost of crashes, reduce project costs, mitigate congestion, and lessen crash severity levels, including those that particularly affect aging drivers.

Page last modified on October 15, 2014
Safe Roads for a Safer Future - Investment in roadway safety saves lives
Federal Highway Administration | 1200 New Jersey Avenue, SE | Washington, DC 20590 | 202-366-4000